Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Neurol. 2010 Jan;67(1):80–86. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.297

Physical Exercise and Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Study

Yonas E Geda 1, Rosebud O Roberts 1, David S Knopman 1, Teresa JH Christianson 1, V Shane Pankratz 1, Robert J Ivnik 1, Bradley F Boeve 1, Eric G Tangalos 1, Ronald C Petersen 1, Walter A Rocca 1
PMCID: PMC2919839  NIHMSID: NIHMS137282  PMID: 20065133

Abstract

Objective

Physical exercise was found to be associated with a decreased risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. We investigated whether physical exercise is also associated with mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Design

Population-based case-control study.

Setting

The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging, an ongoing population-based cohort study in Olmsted County, Minnesota, USA.

Participants

1324 non-demented subjects who completed a questionnaire on physical exercise.

Main Outcome Measures

An expert consensus panel classified each subject as either cognitively normal or affected by MCI using information from a Clinical Dementia Rating Scale administered to the subject and to an informant, a neurological evaluation, and neuropsychological testing to assess 4 cognitive domains.

Results

We compared the frequency of physical exercise in 198 subjects with MCI to the frequency in 1126 cognitively normal subjects and adjusted analyses for age, sex, years of education, medical comorbidity, and depression. The odds ratio (OR) for any frequency of moderate-intensity exercise was 0.61 (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.43–0.88; P=.008) for exercise in midlife (aged 50–65 years), and 0.68 (95% CI, 0.49–0.93; P=.02) for exercise in late life. The findings were consistent in men and women. Light exercise and vigorous exercise were not significantly associated with MCI.

Conclusions

In this population-based case-control study, any frequency of moderate-intensity exercise carried out in either midlife or late life was associated with a reduced OR of MCI.


Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is an intermediate state between the cognitive changes of normal cognitive aging and dementia.16 Subjects with MCI constitute a high risk group because they develop dementia at a rate of 10% to 15% per year compared with 1% to 2% per year in the general population.7 Therefore, it is critical to identify potential protective factors against MCI.

Physical exercise is associated with a reduced risk of heart disease, coronary artery disease, type II diabetes mellitus, some types of cancers, and overall mortality.8,9 A number of observational studies showed that physical exercise may also be protective against dementia and Alzheimer disease1017 with few discrepant findings.18 Two studies also suggested a similar protective effect for MCI.19,20 We investigated whether physical exercise in midlife or proximate to the age at time of onset of MCI is associated with a reduced odds ratio (OR) of MCI in a case-control study derived from the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging.21

METHODS

SETTING AND STUDY DESIGN

We conducted a population-based case-control study comparing subjects with MCI with cognitively normal subjects. This study was derived from the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging, which was described in detail elsewhere.21 Briefly, it is a population-based study designed to estimate the prevalence and incidence of MCI in Olmsted County, Minnesota. Elderly individuals were recruited using stratified random sampling from the target population of nearly 10 000 elderly individuals living in Olmsted County on October 1, 2004. The sampling involved equal allocation of men and women in 2 age strata, 70 to 79 years old and 80 to 89 years old. During the first follow-up performed from 2006 through 2008, subjects were asked to complete a self-reported questionnaire on physical exercise; therefore, the sample of this study was restricted to the 1324 non-demented study participants who completed the questionnaire.

DEFINITION OF CASES AND CONTROLS

Each participant in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging underwent a baseline face-to-face evaluation including 3 components: (1) a neurological evaluation by a physician; (2) a risk factor assessment by a nurse or study coordinator; and (3) neuropsychological testing which was interpreted by a neuropsychologist. The interview by the nurse or study coordinator included administration of the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR)22 to the participant and to an informant. The neurological evaluation was performed by a physician and included administration of the Short Test of Mental Status (STMS),23 medical history review, and a complete neurological examination.

Neuropsychological testing was performed using 9 cognitive tests to assess 4 cognitive domains: (1) Memory (Logical Memory-II [delayed recall] and Visual Reproduction-II [delayed recall] from Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, and delayed recall from the Auditory Verbal Learning Test);2427 (2) Executive Function (Trail Making Test B,28 and Digit Symbol Substitution from Wechsler Adult Intelligent Scale-Revised); (3) Language (Boston Naming Test,29 and category fluency);30 and (4) Visuospatial Skills (Picture Completion and Block Design from WAIS-R). We transformed the raw scores on each test into age-adjusted scores using Mayo’s Older American Normative Studies (MOANS) normative data. These adjusted scores were also scaled to have a mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3.2427

Domain scores were obtained for every subject by summing the age-adjusted scores within each domain. Because different numbers of tests were used to compute domain scores (ie, 2 tests for the executive function, language, and visuospatial domains vs 3 for memory), the domain scores were also scaled to allow comparisons across domains. In summary, the performance of a person in a particular domain was measured by comparing the person’s domain score with the score in normals, available from previous normative work in this same population.2427,31,32 However, the final decision about impairment in any cognitive domain was made by consensus agreement between the examining physician, nurse, and neuropsychologist taking into account years of education, prior occupation, and other information.21

We considered as cases all the participants who met the revised Mayo Clinic criteria for MCI: (1) cognitive concern expressed by a physician, informant, participant, or nurse; (2) cognitive impairment in 1 or more domains (executive function, memory, language, or visuospatial); (3) normal functional activities; and (4) not demented.4,5 Subjects with MCI could have a Clinical Dementia Rating Scale score of 0 or 0.5; however, the final diagnosis of MCI was not based exclusively on the clinical dementia rating, but rather on all available data. We considered as controls all the participants who were cognitively normal according to published normative data developed on this community.2427

MEASUREMENT OF PHYSICAL EXERCISE

We studied frequency and intensity of physical exercise using a self-reported questionnaire with ordinal responses. We used questions from 2 previously validated instruments, the 1985 Health Interview Survey,33 and the Minnesota Heart Survey intensity codes.34 The participants were asked directly to provide information about physical exercise carried out within 1 year of the date of cognitive assessment (late life physical exercise), and carried out at ages 50–65 years (midlife physical exercise). The questionnaire inquired about light, moderate, and vigorous exercise.

Light physical exercise was defined as bowling, leisurely walking, stretching, slow dancing, and golfing with a golf cart. Moderate physical exercise was defined as brisk walking, hiking, aerobics, strength training, golfing without a golf cart, swimming, tennis doubles, moderate use of exercise machines (eg, exercise bike), yoga, martial arts, and weight lifting. Vigorous physical exercise was defined as jogging, backpacking, bicycling uphill, tennis singles, racquetball, intense/extended use of exercise machines, and skiing. For each category of intensity, further inquiry was made as to the frequency of exercise (times per month or per week).

MEASUREMENT OF COVARIATES

We considered as covariates age, sex, years of education, medical comorbidity, and depression. We measured medical comorbidity using the weighted Charlson index, which takes into account both number and severity of diseases (range, 0 to 33).35 We measured depression using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI–II).36

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

We conducted a set of primary analyses considering only intensity of physical exercise to test whether any frequency of exercise was associated with MCI. The “once a month or less” category served as the reference. Prompted by the results of the primary analyses, we also conducted a set of secondary analyses considering both intensity and frequency. The strength of the association between physical exercise and MCI was measured using ORs and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) after adjusting for age (continuous variable), sex, years of education (continuous variable), medical comorbidity (weighted Charlson index as a continuous variable), and depression (BDI-II score <13 vs ≥13).

Analyses were conducted separately for physical exercise carried out at ages 50–65 years, and for physical exercise carried out within 1 year of the date of cognitive assessment. We considered 3 levels of intensity of exercise (light, moderate, and vigorous) and 6 levels of frequency of exercise (once a month or less, 2 to 3 times per month, 1 to 2 times per week, 3 to 4 times per week, 5 to 6 times per week, and daily exercise). Since the 3 categories of intensity of exercise were not mutually exclusive, it was not possible to collapse these categories.

We also conducted a set of sensitivity analyses using a composite score obtained by assigning a numeric score to frequency of physical exercise and adding the scores across the light, moderate, and vigorous strata (equal weighting to all strata). The scores were 0 for once a month or less, 0.5 for 2 to 3 times per month, 1.5 for 1 to 2 times per week, 3.5 for 3 to 4 times per week, 5.5 for 5 to 6 times per week, and 7 for daily. The total composite score ranged between 0 and 21. We consider these analyses as secondary (sensitivity analyses) because the results varied noticeably depending on the assumptions made in computing the scores (weights assigned to different responses).

Statistical testing was done at the conventional 2-tailed alpha level of 0.05. All analyses were performed by using SAS®, version 8 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

RESULTS

CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDY SUBJECTS

Table 1 summarizes the demographic characteristics of the 198 subjects with MCI and the 1126 subjects with normal cognition. Among the cognitively normal subjects, there were an equal number of men and women, whereas among the MCI group there were more men. The median age was 83 years (interquartile range, 78 to 86 years) for the MCI group and 80 years (interquartile range, 76 to 84 years) for the cognitively normal group.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of Subjects with MCI and Controls

MCI
(n=198)
Cognitively Normal
(n=1126)
Variable n (%) n (%) P Value
Men 117 (59.1) 564 (50.1) .02
Age, ya 83 (78–86) 80 (76–84) <.001
    70–79 61 (30.8) 532 (47.2)
    80–93 137 (69.2) 594 (52.8)
Education, ya 12 (12–16) 13 (12–16) .001
    >12 y 92 (46.5) 652 (57.9)
BDI-II depression, total ≥13b 30 (15.2) 62 (5.5) <.001
Weighted Charlson indexa 3 (2–6) 2 (1–5) <.001

Abbreviations: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; NPI, Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire.

a

Median (interquartile range).

b

One patient missing BDI (1 MCI).

RELIABILITY OF THE PHYSICAL EXERCISE QUESTIONNAIRE

We assessed the reliability of the physical exercise questionnaire in 2 ways. First, we studied its internal consistency using Cronbach’s alpha and observed a value of 0.71 (in the moderate to good range). Second, in a sub-sample of 87 subjects who completed the questionnaire at 2 successive visits, we computed a test-retest Spearman’s correlation coefficient. The correlation in the overall group was 0.47 for light exercise, 0.50 for moderate exercise, and 0.33 for vigorous exercise. Interestingly, the test-retest correlations were similar in the 73 cognitively normal subjects and in the 14 subjects affected by MCI (out of the 87 subjects with 2 interviews; data not shown).

PRIMARY ANALYSES STRATIFIED BY INTENSITY OF EXERCISE

Table 2 shows results of primary analyses dichotomizing physical exercise into any frequency of exercise vs none. Light intensity exercise had an OR of 0.90 (95% CI, 0.55–1.47; P=.68) for midlife and 0.69 (95% CI, 0.47–1.00; P=.048) for late life. Moderate intensity exercise had an OR of 0.61 (95% CI, 0.43–0.88; P=.008) for midlife and 0.68 (95% CI, 0.49–0.93; P=.02) for late life. The findings were consistent for men and women (Table 2, footnotes b and c). Vigorous exercise had an OR of 0.82 (95% CI, 0.59–1.15; P=.25) for midlife and 1.14 (95% CI, 0.72–1.81; P=.58) for late life. As expected, relatively fewer people reported vigorous exercise in late life; therefore, the non-significant associations for this analysis may be caused in part by lack of statistical power. Results using a composite score of physical exercise were comparable (Table 2, footnote a).

Table 2.

Primary Analyses for Any Frequency of Exercise Versus None (once a month or less)

MCI
(n=198)
Cognitively Normal
(n=1126)
Intensity Frequency n (%) n (%) OR (95% CI)a P Value
Physical exercise carried out between ages 50 years and 65 years (midlife)
Light None (once a month or less) 25 (12.6) 104 (9.2) 1.00 (reference)
Any frequency 173 (87.4) 1022 (90.8) 0.90 (0.55–1.47) .68
Moderate None (once a month or less) 58 (29.3) 193 (17.1) 1.00 (reference)
Any frequency 140 (70.7) 933 (82.9) 0.61 (0.43–0.88) .008b
Vigorous None (once a month or less) 127 (64.1) 670 (59.5) 1.00 (reference)
Any frequency 71 (35.9) 456 (40.5) 0.82 (0.59–1.15) .25
Physical exercise carried out within the past year (late life)
Light None (once a month or less) 52 (26.3) 184 (16.3) 1.00 (reference)
Any frequency 146 (73.7) 942 (83.7) 0.69 (0.47–1.00) .048
Moderate None (once a month or less) 103 (52.0) 426 (37.8) 1.00 (reference)
Any frequency 95 (48.0) 700 (62.2) 0.68 (0.49–0.93) .02c
Vigorous None (once a month or less) 171 (86.4) 969 (86.1) 1.00 (reference)
Any frequency 27 (13.6) 157 (13.9) 1.14 (0.72–1.81) .58

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.

a

Adjusted for age (continuous variable), sex, years of education (continuous variable), medical comorbidity (weighted Charlson index as a continuous variable), and depression (BDI-II score <13 vs ≥13). In a set of sensitivity analyses using a composite score to combine frequency and intensity of physical exercise, the OR for quartile 4 vs quartile 1 of the composite score distribution was 0.83 (95% CI, 0.54–1.28; P=.40) for midlife and 0.74 (95% CI, 0.45–1.21; P=.23) for late life exercise.

b

The findings were consistent for men (OR, 0.56; 95% CI, 0.34–0.91; P=.02) and for women (OR, 0.68; 95% CI, 0.39–1.17; P=.16). A test for interaction between moderate exercise and age at time of cognitive assessment (continuous variable) was not significant (P=.33).

c

The findings were consistent for men (OR, 0.69; 95% CI, 0.45–1.06; P=.09) and for women (OR, 0.65; 95% CI, 0.39–1.08; P=.10).

SECONDARY ANALYSES CONSIDERING BOTH INTENSITY AND FREQUENCY OF EXERCISE

Table 3 shows our case-control analyses considering both intensity and frequency for physical exercise carried out in midlife (aged 50–65 years). The point estimates for nearly all frequencies of both light-intensity and vigorous-intensity exercise were between 0 and 1, suggesting a potential “protective” effect. However, none of these associations were statistically significant. On the other hand, several frequencies of moderate-intensity exercise were significantly associated with MCI. Table 4 shows our case-control analyses considering both intensity and frequency for physical exercise carried out in late life (within 1 year of the date of cognitive assessment). There was no significant association between physical exercise and MCI in any of the analyses except 1.

Table 3.

Secondary Analyses for Physical Exercise Carried Out at Ages 50–65 Years

MCI
(n=198)
Cognitively Normal
(n=1126)
Intensity Frequency n (%) n (%) OR (95% CI)a P Value
Light None (once a month or less) 25 (12.6) 104 (9.2) 1.00 (reference)
2–3 times per month 18 (9.1) 103 (9.1) 0.83 (0.42–1.67) .61
1–2 times per week 34 (17.2) 211 (18.7) 0.83 (0.46–1.50) .53
3–4 times per week 31 (15.7) 206 (18.3) 0.76 (0.42–1.38) .36
5–6 times per week 26 (13.1) 163 (14.5) 0.94 (0.50–1.76) .84
Every day 64 (32.3) 339 (30.1) 1.07 (0.62–1.83) .82
Moderate None (once a month or less) 58 (29.3) 193 (17.1) 1.00 (reference)
2–3 times per month 19 (9.6) 140 (12.4) 0.56 (0.32–1.01) .05
1–2 times per week 31 (15.7) 216 (19.2) 0.55 (0.34–0.91) .02
3–4 times per week 41 (20.7) 244 (21.7) 0.71 (0.45–1.12) .14
5–6 times per week 16 (8.1) 162 (14.4) 0.42 (0.23–0.77) .005
Every day 33 (16.7) 171 (15.2) 0.76 (0.47–1.24) .28
Vigorous None (once a month or less) 127 (64.1) 670 (59.5) 1.00 (reference)
2–3 times per month 17 (8.6) 142 (12.6) 0.62 (0.35–1.09) .10
1–2 times per week 15 (7.6) 119 (10.6) 0.69 (0.38–1.24) .21
3–4 times per week 14 (7.1) 100 (8.9) 0.84 (0.46–1.55) .59
5–6 times per week 10 (5.1) 44 (3.9) 1.07 (0.51–2.24) .86
Every day 15 (7.6) 51 (4.5) 1.31 (0.70–2.47) .40

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.

a

Adjusted for age (continuous variable), sex, years of education (continuous variable), medical comorbidity (weighted Charlson index as a continuous variable), and depression (BDI-II score <13 vs ≥13).

Table 4.

Secondary Analyses for Physical Exercise Carried Out Within 1 Year of Assessment

MCI
(n=198)
Cognitively Normal
(n=1126)
Intensity Frequency n (%) n (%) OR (95% CI)a P Value
Light None (once a month or less) 52 (26.3) 184 (16.3) 1.00 (reference)
2–3 times per month 17 (8.6) 87 (7.7) 0.73 (0.39–1.37) .33
1–2 times per week 28 (14.1) 172 (15.3) 0.74 (0.44–1.25) .26
3–4 times per week 25 (12.6) 229 (20.3) 0.48 (0.28–0.82) .007
5–6 times per week 18 (9.1) 143 (12.7) 0.62 (0.34–1.14) .12
Every day 58 (29.3) 311 (27.6) 0.81 (0.53–1.25) .35
Moderate None (once a month or less) 103 (52.0) 426 (37.8) 1.00 (reference)
2–3 times per month 16 (8.1) 106 (9.4) 0.72 (0.40–1.29) .27
1–2 times per week 21 (10.6) 154 (13.7) 0.65 (0.38–1.09) .10
3–4 times per week 23 (11.6) 198 (17.6) 0.63 (0.38–1.04) .07
5–6 times per week 15 (7.6) 115 (10.2) 0.69 (0.37–1.25) .22
Every day 20 (10.1) 127 (11.3) 0.73 (0.43–1.24) .24
Vigorous None (once a month or less) 171 (86.4) 969 (86.1) 1.00 (reference)
2–3 times per month 9 (4.5) 52 (4.6) 1.24 (0.59–2.64) .57
1–2 times per week 5 (2.5) 34 (3.0) 0.89 (0.33–2.40) .81
3–4 times per week 9 (4.5) 42 (3.7) 1.52 (0.71–3.26) .28
5–6 times per week 2 (1.0) 11 (1.0) 1.06 (0.23–4.95) .94
Every day 2 (1.0) 18 (1.6) 0.64 (0.15–2.85) .56

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.

a

Adjusted for age (continuous variable), sex, years of education (continuous variable), medical comorbidity (weighted Charlson index as a continuous variable), and depression (BDI-II score <13 vs ≥13).

COMMENT

In this population-based case-control study, midlife moderate physical exercise was associated with a 39% reduced OR for MCI. Similarly, late life moderate physical exercise was associated with a 32% reduced OR for MCI. The ORs for light and vigorous exercise were also consistently smaller than 1.0 in most primary analyses; however, most of these associations were not statistically significant. This may be due in part to the limited statistical power.

Several observational studies have reported possible beneficial effects of physical exercise in cognitively normal elderly as well as in subjects with dementia and Alzheimer disease.1014,16,17,3742 By contrast, investigators from the Chicago Health and Aging Project reported that physical activity conducted within 2 weeks of the date of cognitive assessment was not associated with a decreased risk of cognitive decline in elderly population.18 That negative finding may have been caused in part by the timing of exercise proximate to the assessment of cognition.

One study reported a suggestive but not significant association between physical activity and reduced risk of amnestic MCI.19 A number of observational studies also reported an association of physical exercise with decreased risk of cognitive decline. Although cognitive decline does not coincide with our definition of MCI, these studies are relevant to the interpretation of our findings. The Nurses’ Health Study involving 18 766 women participants aged 70 to 81 years reported that long term physical activity was associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline.43 Similarly, the Monongahela Valley Independent Elders Survey (MoVIES) reported that higher exercise level (defined as aerobic exercise for 30 or more minutes carried out 3 or more times per week) was associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline.13 The MoVIES study had a complete survey of exercise including duration, intensity and frequency of exercise; however, its outcome measure was limited to a Mini-Mental State Examination score.44 The Canadian Health and Aging Study examined the association of physical exercise with Cognitive Impairment No Dementia (CIND) and dementia in a nested case-control study. Even though CIND is not identical with MCI, CIND and MCI are similar constructs describing the gray zone between normal cognitive aging and dementia. The Canadian investigators reported that physical activity was associated with a 42% reduced risk of CIND.10

Recently, a team of Australian investigators conducted a clinical trial of 170 volunteers aged 50 years and older who reported memory problems but did not meet the criteria for dementia. The participants were randomized to either a program of education and usual care or to a 24-week home-based program of physical activity. Exercise improved cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease including an unspecified number of subjects with MCI. These benefits were observed 6 months after initiation of the physical activity, and were sustained at 12 months after the intervention was discontinued.20

All the above observational studies used retrospective questionnaires and interviews to measure physical exercise; hence, some degree of recall bias is inherent in all of them. However, it is reassuring that a University of San Francisco study that objectively measured physical fitness, reported similar findings. The investigators prospectively followed 349 community dwelling elderly women for a duration of 6 to 8 years. At baseline, they objectively measured physical fitness using a treadmill duration test and a peak oxygen consumption test. They also used the oxygen uptake efficiency slope which is a measure of cardiorespiratory fitness independent of motivation and effort. The investigators observed that subjects who were in the highest tertile of cardiorespiratory fitness experienced less cognitive decline over a 6 years follow-up period.11,40

The findings of our study should be interpreted within the context of the following limitations. The first limitation pertains to study design. Both the exposure (physical exercise) and the outcome (MCI) were measured at a cross-sectional point in time. Therefore, it is difficult to study the direction of causality. The second limitation pertains to the measurement of exercise. As in many other observational studies, we used a self-reported questionnaire to collect exercise data. Such a measurement is prone to recall bias.13,45 Third, few people engaged in vigorous exercise in late life; thus, the statistical power was limited for that analysis.

Our study did not address mechanisms of action. Based on the literature, we can speculate that physical exercise may be directly protective against MCI via increased production of neurotrophic factors,46 increased cerebral blood flow, increased neurogenesis, enhanced neuronal survival, mobilization of gene expression impacting neuronal plasticity,47,48 and decreased risk of cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases.49 A second possibility is that physical exercise may be a marker for a healthy life style. A person who engages in regular physical exercise may also show the same type of discipline in dietary habits, accident prevention, compliance with preventive intervention, compliance with medical care, and similar health promoting behaviors.

In summary, our findings contribute to the growing body of literature that indicates the potentially beneficial relationship between physical exercise and cognition. A future population-based cohort study is needed to confirm whether physical exercise is associated with a decreased risk of incident MCI. The population-based setting will improve generalizability, and the prospective cohort design will strengthen etiologic inferences.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (K01 MH068351, U01 AG006786, K01 AG028573, P50 AG016574, R01 AR030582, and R01 NS033978), by the Robert H. and Clarice Smith and Abigail Van Buren Alzheimer’s Disease Research Program, and by the Harold Amos (RWJ) Medical Faculty Development Program.

Additional Contributions: We would like to thank David A. Mrazek, MD, FRCPsych, for his administrative support in the design and conduct of the K01 MH068351 grant.

Footnotes

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Petersen RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, et al. Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. 2005;352(23):2379–2388. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa050151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.DeCarli C. Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence, prognosis, aetiology, and treatment. Lancet Neurol. 2003;2(1):15–21. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(03)00262-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment--beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mitchell AJ, Shiri-Feshki M. Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia--meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119(4):252–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(12):1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.United States Public Health Service. Physical Activity and Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA; Washington, D.C.; Pittsburgh, PA: U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; President's Council on Physical Fitness and Sports; 1996. Office of the Surgeon General, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (U.S.), President's Council on Physical Fitness and Sports (U.S.) [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Institute of Medicine (U.S.) Dietary Reference Intakes for Energy, Carbohydrate, Fiber, Fat, Fatty Acids, Cholesterol, Protein, and Amino Acids. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press; 2005. Panel on Macronutrients., Institute of Medicine (U.S.). Standing Committee on the Scientific Evaluation of Dietary Reference Intakes. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, MacPherson K, Rockwood K. Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(3):498–504. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.3.498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covinsky K. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161(14):1703–1708. doi: 10.1001/archinte.161.14.1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med. 2006;144(2):73–81. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lytle ME, Vander Bilt J, Pandav RS, Dodge HH, Ganguli M. Exercise level and cognitive decline: the MoVIES project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004;18(2):57–64. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000126614.87955.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Abbott RD, White LR, Ross GW, Masaki KH, Curb JD, Petrovitch H. Walking and dementia in physically capable elderly men. JAMA. 2004;292(12):1447–1453. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Barnes DE, Whitmer RA, Yaffe K. Physical activity and dementia: the need for prevention trials. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2007;35(1):24–29. doi: 10.1097/JES.0b013e31802d6bc2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003;348(25):2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wilson RS, Mendes De Leon CF, Barnes LL, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2002;287(6):742–748. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.6.742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sturman MT, Morris MC, Mendes de Leon CF, Bienias JL, Wilson RS, Evans DA. Physical activity, cognitive activity, and cognitive decline in a biracial community population. Arch Neurol. 2005;62(11):1750–1754. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.11.1750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Verghese J, LeValley A, Derby C, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. Neurology. 2006;66(6):821–827. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000202520.68987.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027–1037. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS, et al. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging: design and sampling, participation, baseline measures and sample characteristics. Neuroepidemiology. 2008;30(1):58–69. doi: 10.1159/000115751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43(11):2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kokmen E, Smith GE, Petersen RC, Tangalos E, Ivnik RC. The Short Test of Mental Status. Correlations with standardized psychometric testing. Arch Neurol. 1991;48(7):725–728. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1991.00530190071018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, et al. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: WAIS-R Norms for Ages 56 to 97. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992;65 Supplement:1–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Malec JF, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, et al. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: Utility of corrections for age and education for the WAIS-R. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992;6 Suppl:31–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, et al. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: WMS-R norms for ages 56 to 94. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992;6 Suppl:49–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, et al. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: Updated AVLT norms for ages 56 to 97. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992;6 Suppl:83–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills. 1958 December;8:271–276. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lucas JA, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, et al. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: category fluency norms. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998;20(2):194–200. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.2.194.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ivnik RJ. Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE Token, WRAT-R Reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. Clin Neuropsychol. 1996;10(3):262–278. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Lucas JA, et al. Testing normal older people three or four times at 1- to 2-year intervals: defining normal variance. Neuropsychology. 1999;13(1):121–127. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.13.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Moss AJ, Parsons VL. National Center for Health Statistics (U.S.) Vital and Health StatisticsPublic Health Service. Washington: U.S. Government Printing Office; 1986. Sept, Current estimates from the National Health Interview Survey, United States, 1985. Series 10, No. 160. DHHS Pub. No. (PHS) 86-1588.: Publis Health Service. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Folsom AR, Caspersen CJ, Taylor HL, et al. Leisure time physical activity and its relationship to coronary risk factors in a population-based sample. The Minnesota Heart Survey. Am J Epidemiol. 1985;121(4):570–579. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR. A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40(5):373–383. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(87)90171-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory: Manual. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX; Boston, MA: Psychological Corp.; Harcourt Brace; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Shen C, DeKosky ST. Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study. Neurology. 2004;63(1):115–121. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000132523.27540.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(2):125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3(6):343–353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Satariano WA, Tager IB. A longitudinal study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(4):459–465. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51153.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Middleton L, Kirkland S, Rockwood K. Prevention of CIND by physical activity: different impact on VCI-ND compared with MCI. J Neurol Sci. 2008;269(1–2):80–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.04.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Teri L, Gibbons LE, McCurry SM, et al. Exercise plus behavioral management in patients with Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;290(15):2015–2022. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292(12):1454–1461. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Szklo M, Nieto FJ. Epidemiology: Beyond the Basics. Gaithrsburg, MD: Aspen Publishers, Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(11):1166–1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Rogers RL, Meyer JS, Mortel KF. After reaching retirement age physical activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1990;38(2):123–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1990.tb03472.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(1):58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Blumenthal JA, Emery CF, Madden DJ, et al. Effects of exercise training on cardiorespiratory function in men and women older than 60 years of age. Am J Cardiol. 1991;67(7):633–639. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(91)90904-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES